andrew_gelman_stats andrew_gelman_stats-2011 andrew_gelman_stats-2011-757 knowledge-graph by maker-knowledge-mining

757 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Controversy over the Christakis-Fowler findings on the contagion of obesity


meta infos for this blog

Source: html

Introduction: Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler are famous for finding that obesity is contagious. Their claims, which have been received with both respect and skepticism (perhaps we need a new word for this: “respecticism”?) are based on analysis of data from the Framingham heart study, a large longitudinal public-health study that happened to have some social network data (for the odd reason that each participant was asked to provide the name of a friend who could help the researchers locate them if they were to move away during the study period. The short story is that if your close contact became obese, you were likely to become obese also. The long story is a debate about the reliability of this finding (that is, can it be explained by measurement error and sampling variability) and its causal implications. This sort of study is in my wheelhouse, as it were, but I have never looked at the Christakis-Fowler work in detail. Thus, my previous and current comments are more along the line


Summary: the most important sentenses genereted by tfidf model

sentIndex sentText sentNum sentScore

1 Lyons recognizes this, writing, “while the world may indeed work as C&F; say, their studies do not provide evidence to support such claims. [sent-16, score-0.217]

2 ” I wouldn’t go quite so far as to say they don’t provide evidence, but it seems fair to say they don’t provide convincing or compelling evidence. [sent-17, score-0.221]

3 In debates about empirical social science, there is often a tendency to simply accept descriptive claims and move straight to the arguments about their implications. [sent-21, score-0.216]

4 But as I’ve learned in my own research, often the descriptive claims themselves should be disputed. [sent-22, score-0.216]

5 ) So I’d like to separate Lyons’s criticism of the descriptive inferences and the causal implications. [sent-24, score-0.246]

6 The descriptive criticism is that some of Christakis and Fowler’s observed differences are not statistically significant, thus there is some doubt about generalization to the larger population, it could all just be patterns in random noise. [sent-25, score-0.419]

7 The causal criticism is that, if the descriptive patterns do generalize, they could be explained in other ways than contagion. [sent-26, score-0.306]

8 In particular, he notes on page 6 that the difference between significant and non-significant is not itself statistically significant , a point that should be familiar to regular readers of this space. [sent-29, score-0.234]

9 Lyons goes a bit over the top in the conclusion of his article, slamming observational studies and modeling in general. [sent-34, score-0.291]

10 But statistical modeling is important and useful in many many areas of science and engineering. [sent-35, score-0.22]

11 Bob writes the following about models in computational linguistics: Google translate is heavily model based, being derived from IBM’s original statistical translation models. [sent-40, score-0.337]

12 Ad placement is also heavily model based, and works at least as far as Google’s revenue is concerned. [sent-41, score-0.245]

13 All of the speech recognition in everything from call centers to the desktop is heavily model based, and works pretty well judging by the numbers of people using it. [sent-42, score-0.247]

14 Even seemingly slam-dunk models such as simple random sampling are not true with real surveys, nor are randomization models actually true with experiments on real people. [sent-50, score-0.242]

15 ) I think one should step back before slamming any research just cos it’s observational and model based. [sent-53, score-0.2]

16 Once you have good data, you might very well want to model to learn important things. [sent-67, score-0.202]

17 Which involved lots of work, lots of interaction between the science and the data, and lots of checking. [sent-71, score-0.195]

18 It’s easy to write a sentence like, “viewing observational data through the lens of statistical modeling produces new biases, generally unknown and mostly unacknowledged, lurking in mathematical thickets. [sent-78, score-0.316]

19 After all, even if the Framingham results were unambiguously statistically significant, robust to reasonable models of measurement error, and had a clean identification strategy–even then, it’s just one group of people. [sent-84, score-0.27]

20 I conveyed point 5 above to Lyons and he responded that he respected models too but was concerned with models that cannot be tested. [sent-89, score-0.242]


similar blogs computed by tfidf model

tfidf for this blog:

wordName wordTfidf (topN-words)

[('lyons', 0.559), ('fowler', 0.32), ('christakis', 0.301), ('descriptive', 0.134), ('models', 0.121), ('framingham', 0.112), ('modeling', 0.107), ('heavily', 0.087), ('obese', 0.083), ('claims', 0.082), ('convincing', 0.079), ('observational', 0.075), ('criticisms', 0.071), ('provide', 0.071), ('statistical', 0.068), ('significant', 0.067), ('data', 0.066), ('lots', 0.065), ('slamming', 0.064), ('radon', 0.063), ('obesity', 0.063), ('study', 0.061), ('model', 0.061), ('generalization', 0.061), ('reliability', 0.06), ('patterns', 0.06), ('criticism', 0.059), ('nonlinear', 0.057), ('evidence', 0.056), ('statistically', 0.055), ('based', 0.055), ('intro', 0.054), ('works', 0.053), ('causal', 0.053), ('larger', 0.05), ('learn', 0.05), ('lab', 0.049), ('clean', 0.048), ('score', 0.047), ('well', 0.046), ('reasonable', 0.046), ('friend', 0.046), ('work', 0.045), ('studies', 0.045), ('regular', 0.045), ('important', 0.045), ('debate', 0.045), ('placement', 0.044), ('fletcher', 0.044), ('lac', 0.044)]

similar blogs list:

simIndex simValue blogId blogTitle

same-blog 1 0.99999982 757 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Controversy over the Christakis-Fowler findings on the contagion of obesity

Introduction: Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler are famous for finding that obesity is contagious. Their claims, which have been received with both respect and skepticism (perhaps we need a new word for this: “respecticism”?) are based on analysis of data from the Framingham heart study, a large longitudinal public-health study that happened to have some social network data (for the odd reason that each participant was asked to provide the name of a friend who could help the researchers locate them if they were to move away during the study period. The short story is that if your close contact became obese, you were likely to become obese also. The long story is a debate about the reliability of this finding (that is, can it be explained by measurement error and sampling variability) and its causal implications. This sort of study is in my wheelhouse, as it were, but I have never looked at the Christakis-Fowler work in detail. Thus, my previous and current comments are more along the line

2 0.44136253 756 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Christakis-Fowler update

Introduction: After I posted on Russ Lyons’s criticisms of the work of Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler’s work on social networks, several people emailed in with links to related articles. (Nobody wants to comment on the blog anymore; all I get is emails.) Here they are: Political scientists Hans Noel and Brendan Nyhan wrote a paper called “The ‘Unfriending’ Problem: The Consequences of Homophily in Friendship Retention for Causal Estimates of Social Influence” in which they argue that the Christakis-Fowler results are subject to bias because of patterns in the time course of friendships. Statisticians Cosma Shalizi and AT wrote a paper called “Homophily and Contagion Are Generically Confounded in Observational Social Network Studies” arguing that analyses such as those of Christakis and Fowler cannot hope to disentangle different sorts of network effects. And Christakis and Fowler reply to Noel and Nyhan, Shalizi and Thomas, Lyons, and others in an article that begins: H

3 0.40813312 1699 andrew gelman stats-2013-01-31-Fowlerpalooza!

Introduction: Russ Lyons points us to a discussion in Statistics in Medicine of the famous claims by Christakis and Fowler on the contagion of obesity etc. James O’Malley and Christakis and Fowler present the positive case. Andrew Thomas and Tyler VanderWeele present constructive criticism. Christakis and Fowler reply . Coincidentally, a couple weeks ago an epidemiologist was explaining to me the differences between the Framingham Heart Study and the Nurses Health Study and why Framingham got the postmenopausal supplement risks right while Nurses got it wrong. P.S. The journal issue also includes a comment on “A distribution-free test of constant mean in linear mixed effects models.” Wow! I had no idea people still did this sort of thing. How horrible. But I guess that’s what half-life is all about. These ideas last forever, they just become less and less relevant to people.

4 0.28533092 1412 andrew gelman stats-2012-07-10-More questions on the contagion of obesity, height, etc.

Introduction: AT discusses [link broken; see P.P.S. below] a new paper of his that casts doubt on the robustness of the controversial Christakis and Fowler papers. AT writes that he ran some simulations of contagion on social networks and found that (a) in a simple model assuming the contagion of the sort hypothesized by Christakis and Fowler, their procedure would indeed give the sorts of estimates they found in their papers, but (b) in another simple model assuming a different sort of contagion, the C&F; estimation would give indistinguishable estimates. Thus, if you believe AT’s simulation model, C&F;’s procedure cannot statistically distinguish between two sorts of contagion (directional and simultaneous). I have not looked at AT’s paper so I can’t fully comment, but I don’t fully understand his method for simulating network connections. AT uses what he calls a “rewiring” model. This makes sense: as time progresses, we make new friends and lose old ones—but I am confused by the details

5 0.25758579 1952 andrew gelman stats-2013-07-23-Christakis response to my comment on his comments on social science (or just skip to the P.P.P.S. at the end)

Introduction: The other day, Nicholas Christakis wrote an article in the newspaper criticizing academic social science departments: The social sciences have stagnated. . . . This is not only boring but also counterproductive, constraining engagement with the scientific cutting edge and stifling the creation of new and useful knowledge. . . . I’m not suggesting that social scientists stop teaching and investigating classic topics like monopoly power, racial profiling and health inequality. But everyone knows that monopoly power is bad for markets, that people are racially biased and that illness is unequally distributed by social class. There are diminishing returns from the continuing study of many such topics. And repeatedly observing these phenomena does not help us fix them. I disagreed , saying that Christakis wasn’t giving social science research enough credit: I’m no economist so I can let others discuss the bit about “monopoly power is bad for markets.” I assume that the study by

6 0.23422977 2042 andrew gelman stats-2013-09-28-Difficulties of using statistical significance (or lack thereof) to sift through and compare research hypotheses

7 0.14499933 706 andrew gelman stats-2011-05-11-The happiness gene: My bottom line (for now)

8 0.14208391 2142 andrew gelman stats-2013-12-21-Chasing the noise

9 0.13119729 1949 andrew gelman stats-2013-07-21-Defensive political science responds defensively to an attack on social science

10 0.12808926 702 andrew gelman stats-2011-05-09-“Discovered: the genetic secret of a happy life”

11 0.12519538 1721 andrew gelman stats-2013-02-13-A must-read paper on statistical analysis of experimental data

12 0.12401902 1418 andrew gelman stats-2012-07-16-Long discussion about causal inference and the use of hierarchical models to bridge between different inferential settings

13 0.123155 2172 andrew gelman stats-2014-01-14-Advice on writing research articles

14 0.12212247 1695 andrew gelman stats-2013-01-28-Economists argue about Bayes

15 0.11602203 1425 andrew gelman stats-2012-07-23-Examples of the use of hierarchical modeling to generalize to new settings

16 0.11410536 1865 andrew gelman stats-2013-05-20-What happened that the journal Psychological Science published a paper with no identifiable strengths?

17 0.11222506 1878 andrew gelman stats-2013-05-31-How to fix the tabloids? Toward replicable social science research

18 0.11171386 2245 andrew gelman stats-2014-03-12-More on publishing in journals

19 0.11169972 244 andrew gelman stats-2010-08-30-Useful models, model checking, and external validation: a mini-discussion

20 0.10806374 1972 andrew gelman stats-2013-08-07-When you’re planning on fitting a model, build up to it by fitting simpler models first. Then, once you have a model you like, check the hell out of it


similar blogs computed by lsi model

lsi for this blog:

topicId topicWeight

[(0, 0.291), (1, 0.013), (2, 0.0), (3, -0.073), (4, -0.003), (5, 0.007), (6, -0.06), (7, -0.027), (8, 0.039), (9, 0.044), (10, -0.003), (11, 0.013), (12, -0.014), (13, -0.031), (14, -0.034), (15, 0.032), (16, -0.023), (17, 0.005), (18, -0.02), (19, -0.029), (20, -0.018), (21, -0.065), (22, -0.018), (23, 0.022), (24, -0.001), (25, 0.053), (26, 0.027), (27, -0.029), (28, 0.002), (29, -0.022), (30, -0.053), (31, -0.009), (32, -0.036), (33, -0.049), (34, 0.053), (35, 0.068), (36, -0.043), (37, 0.029), (38, -0.027), (39, -0.031), (40, 0.036), (41, -0.01), (42, -0.021), (43, 0.02), (44, -0.003), (45, -0.044), (46, -0.067), (47, 0.002), (48, -0.068), (49, -0.099)]

similar blogs list:

simIndex simValue blogId blogTitle

same-blog 1 0.9576056 757 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Controversy over the Christakis-Fowler findings on the contagion of obesity

Introduction: Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler are famous for finding that obesity is contagious. Their claims, which have been received with both respect and skepticism (perhaps we need a new word for this: “respecticism”?) are based on analysis of data from the Framingham heart study, a large longitudinal public-health study that happened to have some social network data (for the odd reason that each participant was asked to provide the name of a friend who could help the researchers locate them if they were to move away during the study period. The short story is that if your close contact became obese, you were likely to become obese also. The long story is a debate about the reliability of this finding (that is, can it be explained by measurement error and sampling variability) and its causal implications. This sort of study is in my wheelhouse, as it were, but I have never looked at the Christakis-Fowler work in detail. Thus, my previous and current comments are more along the line

2 0.84665078 756 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Christakis-Fowler update

Introduction: After I posted on Russ Lyons’s criticisms of the work of Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler’s work on social networks, several people emailed in with links to related articles. (Nobody wants to comment on the blog anymore; all I get is emails.) Here they are: Political scientists Hans Noel and Brendan Nyhan wrote a paper called “The ‘Unfriending’ Problem: The Consequences of Homophily in Friendship Retention for Causal Estimates of Social Influence” in which they argue that the Christakis-Fowler results are subject to bias because of patterns in the time course of friendships. Statisticians Cosma Shalizi and AT wrote a paper called “Homophily and Contagion Are Generically Confounded in Observational Social Network Studies” arguing that analyses such as those of Christakis and Fowler cannot hope to disentangle different sorts of network effects. And Christakis and Fowler reply to Noel and Nyhan, Shalizi and Thomas, Lyons, and others in an article that begins: H

3 0.80591303 789 andrew gelman stats-2011-07-07-Descriptive statistics, causal inference, and story time

Introduction: Dave Backus points me to this review by anthropologist Mike McGovern of two books by economist Paul Collier on the politics of economic development in Africa. My first reaction was that this was interesting but non-statistical so I’d have to either post it on the sister blog or wait until the 30 days of statistics was over. But then I looked more carefully and realized that this discussion is very relevant to applied statistics. Here’s McGovern’s substantive critique: Much of the fundamental intellectual work in Collier’s analyses is, in fact, ethnographic. Because it is not done very self-consciously and takes place within a larger econometric rhetoric in which such forms of knowledge are dismissed as “subjective” or worse still biased by the political (read “leftist”) agendas of the academics who create them, it is often ethnography of a low quality. . . . Despite the adoption of a Naipaulian unsentimental-dispatches-from-the-trenches rhetoric, the story told in Collier’s

4 0.79750538 1952 andrew gelman stats-2013-07-23-Christakis response to my comment on his comments on social science (or just skip to the P.P.P.S. at the end)

Introduction: The other day, Nicholas Christakis wrote an article in the newspaper criticizing academic social science departments: The social sciences have stagnated. . . . This is not only boring but also counterproductive, constraining engagement with the scientific cutting edge and stifling the creation of new and useful knowledge. . . . I’m not suggesting that social scientists stop teaching and investigating classic topics like monopoly power, racial profiling and health inequality. But everyone knows that monopoly power is bad for markets, that people are racially biased and that illness is unequally distributed by social class. There are diminishing returns from the continuing study of many such topics. And repeatedly observing these phenomena does not help us fix them. I disagreed , saying that Christakis wasn’t giving social science research enough credit: I’m no economist so I can let others discuss the bit about “monopoly power is bad for markets.” I assume that the study by

5 0.78951132 1949 andrew gelman stats-2013-07-21-Defensive political science responds defensively to an attack on social science

Introduction: Nicholas Christakis, a medical scientist perhaps best known for his controversial claim (see also here ), based on joint work with James Fowler, that obesity is contagious, writes : The social sciences have stagnated. They offer essentially the same set of academic departments and disciplines that they have for nearly 100 years: sociology, economics, anthropology, psychology and political science. This is not only boring but also counterproductive, constraining engagement with the scientific cutting edge and stifling the creation of new and useful knowledge. . . . I’m not suggesting that social scientists stop teaching and investigating classic topics like monopoly power, racial profiling and health inequality. But everyone knows that monopoly power is bad for markets, that people are racially biased and that illness is unequally distributed by social class. There are diminishing returns from the continuing study of many such topics. And repeatedly observing these phenomen

6 0.78496474 2361 andrew gelman stats-2014-06-06-Hurricanes vs. Himmicanes

7 0.7813133 706 andrew gelman stats-2011-05-11-The happiness gene: My bottom line (for now)

8 0.77599347 1412 andrew gelman stats-2012-07-10-More questions on the contagion of obesity, height, etc.

9 0.77303517 2042 andrew gelman stats-2013-09-28-Difficulties of using statistical significance (or lack thereof) to sift through and compare research hypotheses

10 0.769467 145 andrew gelman stats-2010-07-13-Statistical controversy regarding human rights violations in Colomnbia

11 0.76668739 1690 andrew gelman stats-2013-01-23-When are complicated models helpful in psychology research and when are they overkill?

12 0.7607131 2241 andrew gelman stats-2014-03-10-Preregistration: what’s in it for you?

13 0.76065171 556 andrew gelman stats-2011-02-04-Patterns

14 0.75983667 1968 andrew gelman stats-2013-08-05-Evidence on the impact of sustained use of polynomial regression on causal inference (a claim that coal heating is reducing lifespan by 5 years for half a billion people)

15 0.75864023 32 andrew gelman stats-2010-05-14-Causal inference in economics

16 0.75813675 1876 andrew gelman stats-2013-05-29-Another one of those “Psychological Science” papers (this time on biceps size and political attitudes among college students)

17 0.755867 1680 andrew gelman stats-2013-01-18-“If scientists wrote horoscopes, this is what yours would say”

18 0.74682051 382 andrew gelman stats-2010-10-30-“Presidential Election Outcomes Directly Influence Suicide Rates”

19 0.7467503 1889 andrew gelman stats-2013-06-08-Using trends in R-squared to measure progress in criminology??

20 0.74467975 1763 andrew gelman stats-2013-03-14-Everyone’s trading bias for variance at some point, it’s just done at different places in the analyses


similar blogs computed by lda model

lda for this blog:

topicId topicWeight

[(9, 0.018), (15, 0.032), (16, 0.069), (21, 0.036), (24, 0.129), (41, 0.013), (42, 0.012), (45, 0.012), (53, 0.011), (55, 0.027), (76, 0.026), (77, 0.01), (86, 0.019), (89, 0.016), (98, 0.01), (99, 0.436)]

similar blogs list:

simIndex simValue blogId blogTitle

same-blog 1 0.99427861 757 andrew gelman stats-2011-06-10-Controversy over the Christakis-Fowler findings on the contagion of obesity

Introduction: Nicholas Christakis and James Fowler are famous for finding that obesity is contagious. Their claims, which have been received with both respect and skepticism (perhaps we need a new word for this: “respecticism”?) are based on analysis of data from the Framingham heart study, a large longitudinal public-health study that happened to have some social network data (for the odd reason that each participant was asked to provide the name of a friend who could help the researchers locate them if they were to move away during the study period. The short story is that if your close contact became obese, you were likely to become obese also. The long story is a debate about the reliability of this finding (that is, can it be explained by measurement error and sampling variability) and its causal implications. This sort of study is in my wheelhouse, as it were, but I have never looked at the Christakis-Fowler work in detail. Thus, my previous and current comments are more along the line

2 0.99340761 2170 andrew gelman stats-2014-01-13-Judea Pearl overview on causal inference, and more general thoughts on the reexpression of existing methods by considering their implicit assumptions

Introduction: This material should be familiar to many of you but could be helpful to newcomers. Pearl writes: ALL causal conclusions in nonexperimental settings must be based on untested, judgmental assumptions that investigators are prepared to defend on scientific grounds. . . . To understand what the world should be like for a given procedure to work is of no lesser scientific value than seeking evidence for how the world works . . . Assumptions are self-destructive in their honesty. The more explicit the assumption, the more criticism it invites . . . causal diagrams invite the harshest criticism because they make assumptions more explicit and more transparent than other representation schemes. As regular readers know (for example, search this blog for “Pearl”), I have not got much out of the causal-diagrams approach myself, but in general I think that when there are multiple, mathematically equivalent methods of getting the same answer, we tend to go with the framework we are used

3 0.99239069 2245 andrew gelman stats-2014-03-12-More on publishing in journals

Introduction: I’m postponing today’s scheduled post (“Empirical implications of Empirical Implications of Theoretical Models”) to continue the lively discussion from yesterday, What if I were to stop publishing in journals? . An example: my papers with Basbøll Thomas Basbøll and I got into a long discussion on our blogs about business school professor Karl Weick and other cases of plagiarism copying text without attribution. We felt it useful to take our ideas to the next level and write them up as a manuscript, which ended up being logical to split into two papers. At that point I put some effort into getting these papers published, which I eventually did: To throw away data: Plagiarism as a statistical crime went into American Scientist and When do stories work? Evidence and illustration in the social sciences will appear in Sociological Methods and Research. The second paper, in particular, took some effort to place; I got some advice from colleagues in sociology as to where

4 0.99234945 390 andrew gelman stats-2010-11-02-Fragment of statistical autobiography

Introduction: I studied math and physics at MIT. To be more precise, I started in math as default–ever since I was two years old, I’ve thought of myself as a mathematician, and I always did well in math class, so it seemed like a natural fit. But I was concerned. In high school I’d been in the U.S. Mathematical Olympiad training program, and there I’d met kids who were clearly much much better at math than I was. In retrospect, I don’t think I was as bad as I’d thought at the time: there were 24 kids in the program, and I was probably around #20, if that, but I think a lot of the other kids had more practice working on “math olympiad”-type problems. Maybe I was really something like the tenth-best in the group. Tenth-best or twentieth-best, whatever it was, I reached a crisis of confidence around my sophomore or junior year in college. At MIT, I started right off taking advanced math classes, and somewhere along the way I realized I wasn’t seeing the big picture. I was able to do the homework pr

5 0.99234271 2258 andrew gelman stats-2014-03-21-Random matrices in the news

Introduction: From 2010 : Mark Buchanan wrote  a cover article  for the New Scientist on random matrices, a heretofore obscure area of probability theory that his headline writer characterizes as “the deep law that shapes our reality.” It’s interesting stuff, and he gets into some statistical applications at the end, so I’ll give you my take on it. But first, some background. About two hundred years ago, the mathematician/physicist Laplace discovered what is now called the central limit theorem, which is that, under certain conditions, the average of a large number of small random variables has an approximate normal (bell-shaped) distribution. A bit over 100 years ago, social scientists such as Galton applied this theorem to all sorts of biological and social phenomena. The central limit theorem, in its generality, is also important in the information that it indirectly conveys when it fails. For example, the distribution of the heights of adult men or women is nicely bell-shaped, but the

6 0.99232042 1289 andrew gelman stats-2012-04-29-We go to war with the data we have, not the data we want

7 0.99206507 1469 andrew gelman stats-2012-08-25-Ways of knowing

8 0.9920485 702 andrew gelman stats-2011-05-09-“Discovered: the genetic secret of a happy life”

9 0.99199593 2286 andrew gelman stats-2014-04-08-Understanding Simpson’s paradox using a graph

10 0.99137181 2236 andrew gelman stats-2014-03-07-Selection bias in the reporting of shaky research

11 0.99136972 2151 andrew gelman stats-2013-12-27-Should statistics have a Nobel prize?

12 0.9913165 1972 andrew gelman stats-2013-08-07-When you’re planning on fitting a model, build up to it by fitting simpler models first. Then, once you have a model you like, check the hell out of it

13 0.99125189 2327 andrew gelman stats-2014-05-09-Nicholas Wade and the paradox of racism

14 0.9911797 1635 andrew gelman stats-2012-12-22-More Pinker Pinker Pinker

15 0.99103564 1832 andrew gelman stats-2013-04-29-The blogroll

16 0.99097502 1750 andrew gelman stats-2013-03-05-Watership Down, thick description, applied statistics, immutability of stories, and playing tennis with a net

17 0.99096769 2008 andrew gelman stats-2013-09-04-Does it matter that a sample is unrepresentative? It depends on the size of the treatment interactions

18 0.99095315 1807 andrew gelman stats-2013-04-17-Data problems, coding errors…what can be done?

19 0.99079281 451 andrew gelman stats-2010-12-05-What do practitioners need to know about regression?

20 0.9907437 1372 andrew gelman stats-2012-06-08-Stop me before I aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!