nips nips2010 nips2010-98 knowledge-graph by maker-knowledge-mining

98 nips-2010-Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception


Source: pdf

Author: Hongjing Lu, Tungyou Lin, Alan Lee, Luminita Vese, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: It has been speculated that the human motion system combines noisy measurements with prior expectations in an optimal, or rational, manner. The basic goal of our work is to discover experimentally which prior distribution is used. More specifically, we seek to infer the functional form of the motion prior from the performance of human subjects on motion estimation tasks. We restricted ourselves to priors which combine three terms for motion slowness, first-order smoothness, and second-order smoothness. We focused on two functional forms for prior distributions: L2-norm and L1-norm regularization corresponding to the Gaussian and Laplace distributions respectively. In our first experimental session we estimate the weights of the three terms for each functional form to maximize the fit to human performance. We then measured human performance for motion tasks and found that we obtained better fit for the L1-norm (Laplace) than for the L2-norm (Gaussian). We note that the L1-norm is also a better fit to the statistics of motion in natural environments. In addition, we found large weights for the second-order smoothness term, indicating the importance of high-order smoothness compared to slowness and lower-order smoothness. To validate our results further, we used the best fit models using the L1-norm to predict human performance in a second session with different experimental setups. Our results showed excellent agreement between human performance and model prediction – ranging from 3% to 8% for five human subjects over ten experimental conditions – and give further support that the human visual system uses an L1-norm (Laplace) prior.

Reference: text


Summary: the most important sentenses genereted by tfidf model

sentIndex sentText sentNum sentScore

1 Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception Hongjing Lu 1,2 hongjing@ucla. [sent-1, score-1.597]

2 edu Department of Psychology1, Statistics2 , Mathematics3 and Computer Science4 , UCLA Abstract It has been speculated that the human motion system combines noisy measurements with prior expectations in an optimal, or rational, manner. [sent-11, score-0.985]

3 More specifically, we seek to infer the functional form of the motion prior from the performance of human subjects on motion estimation tasks. [sent-13, score-1.676]

4 We restricted ourselves to priors which combine three terms for motion slowness, first-order smoothness, and second-order smoothness. [sent-14, score-0.69]

5 In our first experimental session we estimate the weights of the three terms for each functional form to maximize the fit to human performance. [sent-16, score-0.585]

6 We then measured human performance for motion tasks and found that we obtained better fit for the L1-norm (Laplace) than for the L2-norm (Gaussian). [sent-17, score-0.84]

7 We note that the L1-norm is also a better fit to the statistics of motion in natural environments. [sent-18, score-0.63]

8 In addition, we found large weights for the second-order smoothness term, indicating the importance of high-order smoothness compared to slowness and lower-order smoothness. [sent-19, score-0.448]

9 To validate our results further, we used the best fit models using the L1-norm to predict human performance in a second session with different experimental setups. [sent-20, score-0.522]

10 Our results showed excellent agreement between human performance and model prediction – ranging from 3% to 8% for five human subjects over ten experimental conditions – and give further support that the human visual system uses an L1-norm (Laplace) prior. [sent-21, score-0.893]

11 Your visual system can readily perceive the walking person against the apparently moving background using only the motion signals visible through these holes. [sent-23, score-0.855]

12 But this task is far from trivial due to the inherent local ambiguity of motion stimuli, often referred to as the aperture problem. [sent-24, score-0.709]

13 More precisely, if you view a line segment through an aperture then you can easily estimate the motion component normal to the line but it is impossible to estimate the tangential component. [sent-25, score-0.678]

14 So there are an infinite number of possible interpretations of the local motion signal. [sent-26, score-0.661]

15 One way to overcome this local ambiguity is to integrate local motion measurements across space to infer the ”true” motion field. [sent-27, score-1.418]

16 Then the visual system integrates these local motion measurements to form global motion perception [4, 5]. [sent-29, score-1.562]

17 Psychophysicists have identified a variety of phenomena, such as motion capture and motion cooperativity, which appear to be consequences of motion spatial integration [1, 2, 3]. [sent-30, score-1.951]

18 From the computational perspective, a number of Bayesian models have been proposed to explain these effects by hypothesizing prior assumptions about the motion fields that occur in natural environments. [sent-31, score-0.695]

19 In particular, it has been shown that a prior which is biased to slow-and-smooth motion can account for a range of experimental results [6, 7, 8, 9, 10]. [sent-32, score-0.721]

20 But although evidence from physiology and psychophysics supports the existence of an integration stage, it remains unclear exactly what motion priors are used to resolve the measurement ambiguities. [sent-33, score-0.73]

21 In the walking example described above (see figure 1), the visual system needs to integrate the local measurements in the two regions within the red boxes in order to perceive a coherently moving background. [sent-34, score-0.35]

22 Hence the motion priors used by the human visual system must have a functional form which enables flexible and robust integration. [sent-36, score-1.069]

23 We aim to determine the functional form of the motion priors which underly human perception, and to validate how well these priors can influence human perception in various motion tasks. [sent-37, score-1.927]

24 Our approach is to combine parametric modeling of the motion priors with psychophysical experiments to estimate the model parameters that provide the best fit to human performance across a range of stimulus conditions. [sent-38, score-1.041]

25 The visual system needs to integrate motion measurements from the two regions in the red boxes in order to perceive the motion of the background. [sent-45, score-1.521]

26 2 Functional form of motion priors Many models have proposed that the human visual system uses prior knowledge of probable motions, but the functional form for this prior remains unclear. [sent-49, score-1.199]

27 For example, several well-established computational models employ Gaussian priors to encode the bias towards slow and spatially smooth motion fields. [sent-50, score-0.69]

28 Researchers have used motion sequences in real scenes to measure the spatial and temporal statistics of motion fields [11, 12]. [sent-53, score-1.281]

29 These natural statistics show that the magnitude of the motion (speed) falls off in a manner similar to a Laplacian distribution ( L1-norm regularization), which has heavier tails than Gaussian distributions (see the left plot in figure 2). [sent-54, score-0.727]

30 A similar distribution pattern was also found for spatial derivatives of the motion flow, showing that non-smooth motion fields can also happen in natural environments. [sent-56, score-1.281]

31 This statistical finding is not surprising since motion discontinuities can arise in the natural environment due to the relative motion of objects, foreground/background segmentation, and occlusion. [sent-57, score-1.26]

32 Stocker and Simoncelli [10] conducted a pioneering study to infer the functional form of the slowness motion prior. [sent-58, score-0.873]

33 More specifically, they used human subject responses in a speed discrimination task to infer the shape of the slowness prior distribution. [sent-59, score-0.483]

34 They showed that a motion model using this inferred prior provided an adequate fit to human data for a wide range of stimuli. [sent-61, score-0.93]

35 We illustrate this for motion estimation by the example in the right panel of figure 2. [sent-69, score-0.652]

36 If there is a motion boundary in the true motion field, then a model using L2-norm regularization (Gaussian priors) tends to impose strong smoothing over the two distinct motion fields which blurs the motion across discontinuity. [sent-70, score-2.575]

37 But the model with an L1-norm (Laplace prior) preserves the motion discontinuity and gives smooth motion flow on both sides of it. [sent-71, score-1.285]

38 3 Mathematical Model The input data is specified by local motion measurements rq , of form uq = (u1 q , u2 q ), at a discrete set of positions rq , q = 1, . [sent-74, score-1.052]

39 The goal is to find a smooth motion field v defined at all positions r in the image domain, estimated from the local motion measurements. [sent-78, score-1.291]

40 The motion field v can be thought of as an interpolation of the data which obeys a slowness and smoothness prior and which agrees approximately with the local motion measurements. [sent-79, score-1.649]

41 Recall that u the visual system can only observe the local motion in the directions nq = |uq | (sometimes called q component motion) because of the aperture problem. [sent-80, score-0.964]

42 Hence approximate agreement with local measurements reduces to the constraints: v(rq ) · nq − uq · nq ≈ 0. [sent-81, score-0.419]

43 As illustrated in figure 3, we consider three motion prior terms which quantify the preference for slowness, first-order smoothness and second-order smoothness respectively. [sent-82, score-0.959]

44 More precisely, we initialize ∂t ∂v(r,t) v(r, 0) at random, and solve the update equation for t > 0: ∂vk (r, t) ∂t = −λ|v|α−2 vk + µdiv | v|β−2 vk − η | v|γ−2 vk p−1 − c v(rq ) · nq − uq · nq nk q δr,rq , where k = 1, 2, δr,rq = 1 if r = rq and δr,rq = 0 if r = rq . [sent-97, score-1.641]

45 4 Experiments We compared two possible functional forms for the motion prior: (1) the Laplace distribution with L1-norm regularization, with α = β = γ = 1, (2) the Gaussian distribution with L2-norm regularization, with α = β = γ = 2. [sent-110, score-0.69]

46 Since the main goal of this work is to discover motion priors, we employed the same likelihood term with p = 2 for both models. [sent-111, score-0.63]

47 We used the performance of human subjects in the first experimental session to estimate the weights of the three prior terms, λ, µ, η, for each functional form. [sent-112, score-0.709]

48 We then validated the predictions of the model by comparing them with human performance in a second experimental session which uses different stimulus parameters. [sent-113, score-0.646]

49 The motion stimulus included 20 time frames which were presented within 267 ms. [sent-121, score-0.712]

50 Second, a global motion (also called 2D motion, with the speed of 1 deg/sec) direction was chosen. [sent-124, score-0.698]

51 Third, a certain proportion of elements (signal elements) were assigned with the predetermined 2D motion , while each of the remaining elements (noise elements) was assigned a random 2D motion. [sent-125, score-0.678]

52 Finally, with its orientation and 2D motion velocity, the drifting speed for each element was computed so that the local (or component) drifting velocity was consistent with the assigned 2D motion velocity. [sent-126, score-1.39]

53 As shown in figure 4 the global motion strength was controlled by varying the proportion of signal elements in the stimulus (i. [sent-127, score-0.779]

54 The goal of session 1 was parameter estimation: to estimate the weights of the three prior terms – slowness, first-order smoothness and second-order smoothness, – for each model. [sent-132, score-0.486]

55 Session 2 was for model validation: using the weights estimated from session 1 to predict subject performance for different experimental conditions. [sent-133, score-0.36]

56 the blue and green arrows indicate the 2D motion directions assigned for signal and noise elements, respectively. [sent-138, score-0.738]

57 On each trial of the first session, observers were presented with two motion patterns, one after another. [sent-143, score-0.805]

58 The first one was the reference motion pattern, which always moved upward (0 degree), and the second one was the test motion pattern, whose global motion direction was either tilted towards the left or the right relative to the reference pattern. [sent-144, score-2.025]

59 The observer’s task was to determine whether the global motion direction of the test pattern was more towards the left or right relative to the reference pattern. [sent-146, score-0.719]

60 In order to make sure observers understood the task and were able to perceive the global motion, before the beginning of the first session, observers passed a test session in which they achieved 90% accuracy in 40 consecutive trials with 80% coherence and 20 (or 45) degrees of angular difference. [sent-147, score-0.996]

61 To allow observers to familiarize themselves with the task, before each experimental session observers went through a practice session with 10 blocks of 25 trials. [sent-148, score-0.937]

62 The angular difference between the reference and test motion was fixed for each observer in the entire session (2 degrees for observers AL, MW and AE; 45 degrees for OQ and CC). [sent-157, score-1.372]

63 The second session was identical to the first one, except that the coherence ratio was fixed at 0. [sent-158, score-0.5]

64 7, and the angular difference between the global motion directions of the reference and the test patterns was varied across blocks (ten angular differences: 1, 5, 10, . [sent-159, score-0.983]

65 2 Results We implemented motion models with the Laplace prior distribution (termed ”L1 model”) and the Gaussian prior (termed ”L2 model”). [sent-164, score-0.76]

66 As the first step, exhaustive search was conducted to find a set of weights for the prior terms that provided the best fit to the human psychometric performance in experimental session 1. [sent-165, score-0.59]

67 However, across all five subjects, large weight values were found for the second-order smoothness terms, indicating the contribution from higher-order smoothness preference is important in perceiving global motion from multiple-aperture stimulus. [sent-168, score-0.937]

68 In general,humans appear to be sensitive to the inclusion of noise elements, and perform 6 Table 1: Estimated weights λ, µ, η of slowness, first-order smoothness and second-order smoothness prior terms, for L1 and L2-norm model Subjects L1 λ L1 µ L1 η L2 λ L2 µ L2 η AE AL CC MW OQ 0. [sent-171, score-0.377]

69 In experimental session 2, the two models predicted performance as a function of angular difference between the reference motion and the test motion. [sent-184, score-1.079]

70 This result illustrates the power of the L1 model in predicting human performance in motion tasks different from the tasks used for estimating model parameters. [sent-186, score-0.89]

71 08 0 Coherence ratio AL AE CC MW OQ Figure 5: Comparison between human performance and model predictions in session 1. [sent-226, score-0.594]

72 05 45 0 AL AE CC MW OQ Figure 6: Comparison between human performance and model predictions in session 1. [sent-248, score-0.538]

73 Left two plots, accuracy as a function of angular difference between the reference and the test motion for two representative subjects. [sent-249, score-0.787]

74 Less errors from L1 model indicate that L1 model consistently fits human performance better than L2 model for all subjects 4. [sent-253, score-0.366]

75 3 Experiment 2 The results of Experiment 1 clearly support the conclusion that the motion model with Laplace prior (L1-norm regularization) fits human performance better than does the model with Gaussian prior 7 (L2 model). [sent-254, score-1.02]

76 In Experiment 2, we compared human motion judgment with predictions of the L1 model on each trial, rather than using the average performance as in Experiment 1. [sent-255, score-0.952]

77 Such a detailed comparison can provide quantitative measures of how well the L1 model is able to predict human motion judgment for specific stimuli. [sent-256, score-0.935]

78 In Experiment 2, the first session was identical to that in Experiment 1, in which angular difference in the two global motion directions were fixed (45 degrees for all observers) while the coherence ratio was varied. [sent-257, score-1.352]

79 In the second session, observers were presented with one motion stimulus on each trial. [sent-258, score-0.887]

80 The global motion direction of the pattern was randomly selected from 24 possible directions (with a 15-degree difference between two adjacent directions). [sent-259, score-0.73]

81 Observers reported their perceived global motion directions by rotating a line after the motion stimulus disappeared from the screen. [sent-260, score-1.42]

82 A two-pass design was used to let each observer run the identical session twice in order to measure the reliability of the observer’s judgments. [sent-267, score-0.39]

83 We used human performance in session 1 to estimate model parameters: weights λ, µ, η for slowness, first-order smoothness and second-order smoothness prior terms for each individual participant. [sent-268, score-0.853]

84 Since identical stimuli were used in the two runs of session 2, we can quantify the reliability of the observer’s judgment by computing the response correlation across trials in these two runs. [sent-269, score-0.478]

85 As shown in the left plot of figure 7, human observers’ responses were significantly correlated in the two runs, even in the condition of random motion (coherence ratio is close to 0). [sent-270, score-0.935]

86 The correlated responses in these subthreshold conditions suggest that human observers are able to provide consistent interpretation of motion flow, even when the motion is random. [sent-271, score-1.645]

87 The right plot of figure 7 shows the trial-by-trial correlation between human motion judgments with model-predicted global motion direction. [sent-272, score-1.622]

88 Even in the random motion condition (where the coherence ratio is 0), the correlation between the model and human judgments is greater than 0. [sent-274, score-1.148]

89 We also noticed that the correlation between human and L2 model was around 8 percent worse than the human self-correlation and the correlation between the L1 model and humans. [sent-276, score-0.562]

90 9 Coherence ratio Coherence ratio Figure 7: Comparison between human performance and model predictions using trial-by-trial correlation. [sent-312, score-0.384]

91 Left plot, human self correlation between two runs of identical experimental sessions. [sent-313, score-0.325]

92 Right plot, correlation between human motion judgement and model predicted global motion direction. [sent-314, score-1.584]

93 The significant correlation between human and the model indicates the L1 model is able to predict human motion judgment for specific stimuli, even in the random display, i. [sent-315, score-1.216]

94 5 Conclusions We found that a motion prior in the form of the Laplace distribution with L1-norm regularization provided significantly better agreement with human performance than did Gaussian priors with L2norm. [sent-318, score-1.018]

95 We also showed that humans weighted second-order motion smoothness much higher than first-order smoothness and slowness. [sent-319, score-0.894]

96 Furthermore, model predictions using this Laplace prior were consistent with human perception of coherent motion, even for random displays. [sent-320, score-0.43]

97 Overall our results suggest that human motion perception for these types of stimuli can be well modeled using Laplace priors. [sent-321, score-0.977]

98 How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns. [sent-368, score-0.711]

99 Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception. [sent-411, score-0.381]

100 Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. [sent-434, score-1.03]


similar papers computed by tfidf model

tfidf for this paper:

wordName wordTfidf (topN-words)

[('motion', 0.63), ('vk', 0.36), ('session', 0.266), ('human', 0.21), ('observers', 0.175), ('slowness', 0.161), ('coherence', 0.157), ('smoothness', 0.132), ('ci', 0.128), ('rq', 0.124), ('nq', 0.111), ('laplace', 0.098), ('uq', 0.091), ('angular', 0.089), ('ae', 0.088), ('stimulus', 0.082), ('visual', 0.081), ('observer', 0.078), ('walker', 0.073), ('oq', 0.072), ('cc', 0.071), ('stimuli', 0.07), ('perception', 0.067), ('prior', 0.065), ('priors', 0.06), ('functional', 0.06), ('subjects', 0.059), ('perceive', 0.058), ('ratio', 0.056), ('mw', 0.055), ('idk', 0.054), ('measurements', 0.052), ('judgment', 0.05), ('aperture', 0.048), ('yuille', 0.048), ('correlation', 0.046), ('reference', 0.046), ('global', 0.043), ('integration', 0.04), ('intercept', 0.039), ('plot', 0.039), ('drifting', 0.037), ('predictions', 0.037), ('punch', 0.036), ('grating', 0.036), ('directions', 0.035), ('moving', 0.035), ('psychophysical', 0.034), ('simoncelli', 0.034), ('degrees', 0.033), ('tails', 0.033), ('motions', 0.032), ('gure', 0.032), ('energy', 0.032), ('hongjing', 0.032), ('local', 0.031), ('regularization', 0.03), ('ms', 0.03), ('al', 0.03), ('experiment', 0.03), ('blocks', 0.029), ('neuroscience', 0.029), ('stocker', 0.029), ('pdes', 0.029), ('system', 0.028), ('sessions', 0.027), ('equations', 0.026), ('coherent', 0.026), ('experimental', 0.026), ('gaussian', 0.026), ('blue', 0.026), ('exponents', 0.026), ('green', 0.025), ('model', 0.025), ('speed', 0.025), ('reliability', 0.025), ('heavier', 0.025), ('participant', 0.025), ('sg', 0.025), ('elements', 0.024), ('bi', 0.024), ('judgments', 0.024), ('exhibited', 0.024), ('agreement', 0.023), ('weights', 0.023), ('walking', 0.023), ('panel', 0.022), ('difference', 0.022), ('discretization', 0.022), ('self', 0.022), ('indicate', 0.022), ('integrate', 0.022), ('infer', 0.022), ('ow', 0.021), ('spatial', 0.021), ('identical', 0.021), ('ten', 0.021), ('predict', 0.02), ('boxes', 0.02), ('lu', 0.02)]

similar papers list:

simIndex simValue paperId paperTitle

same-paper 1 1.0000004 98 nips-2010-Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception

Author: Hongjing Lu, Tungyou Lin, Alan Lee, Luminita Vese, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: It has been speculated that the human motion system combines noisy measurements with prior expectations in an optimal, or rational, manner. The basic goal of our work is to discover experimentally which prior distribution is used. More specifically, we seek to infer the functional form of the motion prior from the performance of human subjects on motion estimation tasks. We restricted ourselves to priors which combine three terms for motion slowness, first-order smoothness, and second-order smoothness. We focused on two functional forms for prior distributions: L2-norm and L1-norm regularization corresponding to the Gaussian and Laplace distributions respectively. In our first experimental session we estimate the weights of the three terms for each functional form to maximize the fit to human performance. We then measured human performance for motion tasks and found that we obtained better fit for the L1-norm (Laplace) than for the L2-norm (Gaussian). We note that the L1-norm is also a better fit to the statistics of motion in natural environments. In addition, we found large weights for the second-order smoothness term, indicating the importance of high-order smoothness compared to slowness and lower-order smoothness. To validate our results further, we used the best fit models using the L1-norm to predict human performance in a second session with different experimental setups. Our results showed excellent agreement between human performance and model prediction – ranging from 3% to 8% for five human subjects over ten experimental conditions – and give further support that the human visual system uses an L1-norm (Laplace) prior.

2 0.5753926 20 nips-2010-A unified model of short-range and long-range motion perception

Author: Shuang Wu, Xuming He, Hongjing Lu, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: The human vision system is able to effortlessly perceive both short-range and long-range motion patterns in complex dynamic scenes. Previous work has assumed that two different mechanisms are involved in processing these two types of motion. In this paper, we propose a hierarchical model as a unified framework for modeling both short-range and long-range motion perception. Our model consists of two key components: a data likelihood that proposes multiple motion hypotheses using nonlinear matching, and a hierarchical prior that imposes slowness and spatial smoothness constraints on the motion field at multiple scales. We tested our model on two types of stimuli, random dot kinematograms and multiple-aperture stimuli, both commonly used in human vision research. We demonstrate that the hierarchical model adequately accounts for human performance in psychophysical experiments.

3 0.21113299 187 nips-2010-Occlusion Detection and Motion Estimation with Convex Optimization

Author: Alper Ayvaci, Michalis Raptis, Stefano Soatto

Abstract: We tackle the problem of simultaneously detecting occlusions and estimating optical flow. We show that, under standard assumptions of Lambertian reflection and static illumination, the task can be posed as a convex minimization problem. Therefore, the solution, computed using efficient algorithms, is guaranteed to be globally optimal, for any number of independently moving objects, and any number of occlusion layers. We test the proposed algorithm on benchmark datasets, expanded to enable evaluation of occlusion detection performance. 1

4 0.19877 221 nips-2010-Random Projections for $k$-means Clustering

Author: Christos Boutsidis, Anastasios Zouzias, Petros Drineas

Abstract: This paper discusses the topic of dimensionality reduction for k-means clustering. We prove that any set of n points in d dimensions (rows in a matrix A ∈ Rn×d ) can be projected into t = Ω(k/ε2 ) dimensions, for any ε ∈ (0, 1/3), in O(nd⌈ε−2 k/ log(d)⌉) time, such that with constant probability the optimal k-partition of the point set is preserved within a factor of 2 + √ The projection is done ε. √ by post-multiplying A with a d × t random matrix R having entries +1/ t or −1/ t with equal probability. A numerical implementation of our technique and experiments on a large face images dataset verify the speed and the accuracy of our theoretical results.

5 0.17819068 141 nips-2010-Layered image motion with explicit occlusions, temporal consistency, and depth ordering

Author: Deqing Sun, Erik B. Sudderth, Michael J. Black

Abstract: Layered models are a powerful way of describing natural scenes containing smooth surfaces that may overlap and occlude each other. For image motion estimation, such models have a long history but have not achieved the wide use or accuracy of non-layered methods. We present a new probabilistic model of optical flow in layers that addresses many of the shortcomings of previous approaches. In particular, we define a probabilistic graphical model that explicitly captures: 1) occlusions and disocclusions; 2) depth ordering of the layers; 3) temporal consistency of the layer segmentation. Additionally the optical flow in each layer is modeled by a combination of a parametric model and a smooth deviation based on an MRF with a robust spatial prior; the resulting model allows roughness in layers. Finally, a key contribution is the formulation of the layers using an imagedependent hidden field prior based on recent models for static scene segmentation. The method achieves state-of-the-art results on the Middlebury benchmark and produces meaningful scene segmentations as well as detected occlusion regions.

6 0.10892656 257 nips-2010-Structured Determinantal Point Processes

7 0.10041041 246 nips-2010-Sparse Coding for Learning Interpretable Spatio-Temporal Primitives

8 0.094284706 78 nips-2010-Error Propagation for Approximate Policy and Value Iteration

9 0.088814303 44 nips-2010-Brain covariance selection: better individual functional connectivity models using population prior

10 0.078025699 230 nips-2010-Robust Clustering as Ensembles of Affinity Relations

11 0.077793278 128 nips-2010-Infinite Relational Modeling of Functional Connectivity in Resting State fMRI

12 0.073233217 92 nips-2010-Fast global convergence rates of gradient methods for high-dimensional statistical recovery

13 0.070328057 119 nips-2010-Implicit encoding of prior probabilities in optimal neural populations

14 0.0688117 167 nips-2010-Mixture of time-warped trajectory models for movement decoding

15 0.066100322 55 nips-2010-Cross Species Expression Analysis using a Dirichlet Process Mixture Model with Latent Matchings

16 0.065503433 161 nips-2010-Linear readout from a neural population with partial correlation data

17 0.061079767 245 nips-2010-Space-Variant Single-Image Blind Deconvolution for Removing Camera Shake

18 0.060644872 179 nips-2010-Natural Policy Gradient Methods with Parameter-based Exploration for Control Tasks

19 0.056775067 21 nips-2010-Accounting for network effects in neuronal responses using L1 regularized point process models

20 0.056454021 95 nips-2010-Feature Transitions with Saccadic Search: Size, Color, and Orientation Are Not Alike


similar papers computed by lsi model

lsi for this paper:

topicId topicWeight

[(0, 0.16), (1, 0.038), (2, -0.186), (3, 0.076), (4, -0.008), (5, -0.173), (6, -0.041), (7, 0.019), (8, 0.011), (9, 0.014), (10, 0.155), (11, -0.475), (12, -0.096), (13, 0.197), (14, 0.204), (15, 0.24), (16, -0.204), (17, 0.138), (18, -0.015), (19, 0.012), (20, -0.133), (21, -0.093), (22, 0.0), (23, -0.059), (24, -0.024), (25, 0.05), (26, 0.043), (27, -0.045), (28, 0.042), (29, -0.057), (30, 0.033), (31, -0.056), (32, -0.08), (33, 0.091), (34, 0.171), (35, -0.026), (36, 0.046), (37, 0.008), (38, -0.031), (39, -0.09), (40, -0.015), (41, -0.005), (42, 0.122), (43, 0.057), (44, 0.034), (45, 0.026), (46, 0.028), (47, 0.081), (48, -0.038), (49, -0.039)]

similar papers list:

simIndex simValue paperId paperTitle

same-paper 1 0.99005187 98 nips-2010-Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception

Author: Hongjing Lu, Tungyou Lin, Alan Lee, Luminita Vese, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: It has been speculated that the human motion system combines noisy measurements with prior expectations in an optimal, or rational, manner. The basic goal of our work is to discover experimentally which prior distribution is used. More specifically, we seek to infer the functional form of the motion prior from the performance of human subjects on motion estimation tasks. We restricted ourselves to priors which combine three terms for motion slowness, first-order smoothness, and second-order smoothness. We focused on two functional forms for prior distributions: L2-norm and L1-norm regularization corresponding to the Gaussian and Laplace distributions respectively. In our first experimental session we estimate the weights of the three terms for each functional form to maximize the fit to human performance. We then measured human performance for motion tasks and found that we obtained better fit for the L1-norm (Laplace) than for the L2-norm (Gaussian). We note that the L1-norm is also a better fit to the statistics of motion in natural environments. In addition, we found large weights for the second-order smoothness term, indicating the importance of high-order smoothness compared to slowness and lower-order smoothness. To validate our results further, we used the best fit models using the L1-norm to predict human performance in a second session with different experimental setups. Our results showed excellent agreement between human performance and model prediction – ranging from 3% to 8% for five human subjects over ten experimental conditions – and give further support that the human visual system uses an L1-norm (Laplace) prior.

2 0.9498632 20 nips-2010-A unified model of short-range and long-range motion perception

Author: Shuang Wu, Xuming He, Hongjing Lu, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: The human vision system is able to effortlessly perceive both short-range and long-range motion patterns in complex dynamic scenes. Previous work has assumed that two different mechanisms are involved in processing these two types of motion. In this paper, we propose a hierarchical model as a unified framework for modeling both short-range and long-range motion perception. Our model consists of two key components: a data likelihood that proposes multiple motion hypotheses using nonlinear matching, and a hierarchical prior that imposes slowness and spatial smoothness constraints on the motion field at multiple scales. We tested our model on two types of stimuli, random dot kinematograms and multiple-aperture stimuli, both commonly used in human vision research. We demonstrate that the hierarchical model adequately accounts for human performance in psychophysical experiments.

3 0.64416111 187 nips-2010-Occlusion Detection and Motion Estimation with Convex Optimization

Author: Alper Ayvaci, Michalis Raptis, Stefano Soatto

Abstract: We tackle the problem of simultaneously detecting occlusions and estimating optical flow. We show that, under standard assumptions of Lambertian reflection and static illumination, the task can be posed as a convex minimization problem. Therefore, the solution, computed using efficient algorithms, is guaranteed to be globally optimal, for any number of independently moving objects, and any number of occlusion layers. We test the proposed algorithm on benchmark datasets, expanded to enable evaluation of occlusion detection performance. 1

4 0.53595227 141 nips-2010-Layered image motion with explicit occlusions, temporal consistency, and depth ordering

Author: Deqing Sun, Erik B. Sudderth, Michael J. Black

Abstract: Layered models are a powerful way of describing natural scenes containing smooth surfaces that may overlap and occlude each other. For image motion estimation, such models have a long history but have not achieved the wide use or accuracy of non-layered methods. We present a new probabilistic model of optical flow in layers that addresses many of the shortcomings of previous approaches. In particular, we define a probabilistic graphical model that explicitly captures: 1) occlusions and disocclusions; 2) depth ordering of the layers; 3) temporal consistency of the layer segmentation. Additionally the optical flow in each layer is modeled by a combination of a parametric model and a smooth deviation based on an MRF with a robust spatial prior; the resulting model allows roughness in layers. Finally, a key contribution is the formulation of the layers using an imagedependent hidden field prior based on recent models for static scene segmentation. The method achieves state-of-the-art results on the Middlebury benchmark and produces meaningful scene segmentations as well as detected occlusion regions.

5 0.36043057 221 nips-2010-Random Projections for $k$-means Clustering

Author: Christos Boutsidis, Anastasios Zouzias, Petros Drineas

Abstract: This paper discusses the topic of dimensionality reduction for k-means clustering. We prove that any set of n points in d dimensions (rows in a matrix A ∈ Rn×d ) can be projected into t = Ω(k/ε2 ) dimensions, for any ε ∈ (0, 1/3), in O(nd⌈ε−2 k/ log(d)⌉) time, such that with constant probability the optimal k-partition of the point set is preserved within a factor of 2 + √ The projection is done ε. √ by post-multiplying A with a d × t random matrix R having entries +1/ t or −1/ t with equal probability. A numerical implementation of our technique and experiments on a large face images dataset verify the speed and the accuracy of our theoretical results.

6 0.35506657 245 nips-2010-Space-Variant Single-Image Blind Deconvolution for Removing Camera Shake

7 0.35397214 214 nips-2010-Probabilistic Belief Revision with Structural Constraints

8 0.3379674 246 nips-2010-Sparse Coding for Learning Interpretable Spatio-Temporal Primitives

9 0.32850391 3 nips-2010-A Bayesian Framework for Figure-Ground Interpretation

10 0.28340352 257 nips-2010-Structured Determinantal Point Processes

11 0.26925743 121 nips-2010-Improving Human Judgments by Decontaminating Sequential Dependencies

12 0.25755322 45 nips-2010-CUR from a Sparse Optimization Viewpoint

13 0.25051689 95 nips-2010-Feature Transitions with Saccadic Search: Size, Color, and Orientation Are Not Alike

14 0.24960558 167 nips-2010-Mixture of time-warped trajectory models for movement decoding

15 0.21799572 55 nips-2010-Cross Species Expression Analysis using a Dirichlet Process Mixture Model with Latent Matchings

16 0.20940378 262 nips-2010-Switched Latent Force Models for Movement Segmentation

17 0.20476377 81 nips-2010-Evaluating neuronal codes for inference using Fisher information

18 0.20442872 171 nips-2010-Movement extraction by detecting dynamics switches and repetitions

19 0.20133977 161 nips-2010-Linear readout from a neural population with partial correlation data

20 0.19887453 44 nips-2010-Brain covariance selection: better individual functional connectivity models using population prior


similar papers computed by lda model

lda for this paper:

topicId topicWeight

[(3, 0.012), (13, 0.06), (27, 0.19), (30, 0.044), (35, 0.014), (43, 0.064), (45, 0.203), (50, 0.068), (52, 0.035), (60, 0.019), (75, 0.074), (77, 0.06), (78, 0.015), (90, 0.034)]

similar papers list:

simIndex simValue paperId paperTitle

same-paper 1 0.93584919 98 nips-2010-Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception

Author: Hongjing Lu, Tungyou Lin, Alan Lee, Luminita Vese, Alan L. Yuille

Abstract: It has been speculated that the human motion system combines noisy measurements with prior expectations in an optimal, or rational, manner. The basic goal of our work is to discover experimentally which prior distribution is used. More specifically, we seek to infer the functional form of the motion prior from the performance of human subjects on motion estimation tasks. We restricted ourselves to priors which combine three terms for motion slowness, first-order smoothness, and second-order smoothness. We focused on two functional forms for prior distributions: L2-norm and L1-norm regularization corresponding to the Gaussian and Laplace distributions respectively. In our first experimental session we estimate the weights of the three terms for each functional form to maximize the fit to human performance. We then measured human performance for motion tasks and found that we obtained better fit for the L1-norm (Laplace) than for the L2-norm (Gaussian). We note that the L1-norm is also a better fit to the statistics of motion in natural environments. In addition, we found large weights for the second-order smoothness term, indicating the importance of high-order smoothness compared to slowness and lower-order smoothness. To validate our results further, we used the best fit models using the L1-norm to predict human performance in a second session with different experimental setups. Our results showed excellent agreement between human performance and model prediction – ranging from 3% to 8% for five human subjects over ten experimental conditions – and give further support that the human visual system uses an L1-norm (Laplace) prior.

2 0.93308604 161 nips-2010-Linear readout from a neural population with partial correlation data

Author: Adrien Wohrer, Ranulfo Romo, Christian K. Machens

Abstract: How much information does a neural population convey about a stimulus? Answers to this question are known to strongly depend on the correlation of response variability in neural populations. These noise correlations, however, are essentially immeasurable as the number of parameters in a noise correlation matrix grows quadratically with population size. Here, we suggest to bypass this problem by imposing a parametric model on a noise correlation matrix. Our basic assumption is that noise correlations arise due to common inputs between neurons. On average, noise correlations will therefore reflect signal correlations, which can be measured in neural populations. We suggest an explicit parametric dependency between signal and noise correlations. We show how this dependency can be used to ”fill the gaps” in noise correlations matrices using an iterative application of the Wishart distribution over positive definitive matrices. We apply our method to data from the primary somatosensory cortex of monkeys performing a two-alternativeforced choice task. We compare the discrimination thresholds read out from the population of recorded neurons with the discrimination threshold of the monkey and show that our method predicts different results than simpler, average schemes of noise correlations. 1

3 0.92672455 81 nips-2010-Evaluating neuronal codes for inference using Fisher information

Author: Haefner Ralf, Matthias Bethge

Abstract: Many studies have explored the impact of response variability on the quality of sensory codes. The source of this variability is almost always assumed to be intrinsic to the brain. However, when inferring a particular stimulus property, variability associated with other stimulus attributes also effectively act as noise. Here we study the impact of such stimulus-induced response variability for the case of binocular disparity inference. We characterize the response distribution for the binocular energy model in response to random dot stereograms and find it to be very different from the Poisson-like noise usually assumed. We then compute the Fisher information with respect to binocular disparity, present in the monocular inputs to the standard model of early binocular processing, and thereby obtain an upper bound on how much information a model could theoretically extract from them. Then we analyze the information loss incurred by the different ways of combining those inputs to produce a scalar single-neuron response. We find that in the case of depth inference, monocular stimulus variability places a greater limit on the extractable information than intrinsic neuronal noise for typical spike counts. Furthermore, the largest loss of information is incurred by the standard model for position disparity neurons (tuned-excitatory), that are the most ubiquitous in monkey primary visual cortex, while more information from the inputs is preserved in phase-disparity neurons (tuned-near or tuned-far) primarily found in higher cortical regions. 1

4 0.92261362 266 nips-2010-The Maximal Causes of Natural Scenes are Edge Filters

Author: Jose Puertas, Joerg Bornschein, Joerg Luecke

Abstract: We study the application of a strongly non-linear generative model to image patches. As in standard approaches such as Sparse Coding or Independent Component Analysis, the model assumes a sparse prior with independent hidden variables. However, in the place where standard approaches use the sum to combine basis functions we use the maximum. To derive tractable approximations for parameter estimation we apply a novel approach based on variational Expectation Maximization. The derived learning algorithm can be applied to large-scale problems with hundreds of observed and hidden variables. Furthermore, we can infer all model parameters including observation noise and the degree of sparseness. In applications to image patches we find that Gabor-like basis functions are obtained. Gabor-like functions are thus not a feature exclusive to approaches assuming linear superposition. Quantitatively, the inferred basis functions show a large diversity of shapes with many strongly elongated and many circular symmetric functions. The distribution of basis function shapes reflects properties of simple cell receptive fields that are not reproduced by standard linear approaches. In the study of natural image statistics, the implications of using different superposition assumptions have so far not been investigated systematically because models with strong non-linearities have been found analytically and computationally challenging. The presented algorithm represents the first large-scale application of such an approach. 1

5 0.9211908 39 nips-2010-Bayesian Action-Graph Games

Author: Albert X. Jiang, Kevin Leyton-brown

Abstract: Games of incomplete information, or Bayesian games, are an important gametheoretic model and have many applications in economics. We propose Bayesian action-graph games (BAGGs), a novel graphical representation for Bayesian games. BAGGs can represent arbitrary Bayesian games, and furthermore can compactly express Bayesian games exhibiting commonly encountered types of structure including symmetry, action- and type-specific utility independence, and probabilistic independence of type distributions. We provide an algorithm for computing expected utility in BAGGs, and discuss conditions under which the algorithm runs in polynomial time. Bayes-Nash equilibria of BAGGs can be computed by adapting existing algorithms for complete-information normal form games and leveraging our expected utility algorithm. We show both theoretically and empirically that our approaches improve significantly on the state of the art. 1

6 0.91991341 21 nips-2010-Accounting for network effects in neuronal responses using L1 regularized point process models

7 0.91424304 121 nips-2010-Improving Human Judgments by Decontaminating Sequential Dependencies

8 0.90688205 286 nips-2010-Word Features for Latent Dirichlet Allocation

9 0.90399688 194 nips-2010-Online Learning for Latent Dirichlet Allocation

10 0.9021399 6 nips-2010-A Discriminative Latent Model of Image Region and Object Tag Correspondence

11 0.90196067 60 nips-2010-Deterministic Single-Pass Algorithm for LDA

12 0.89615005 20 nips-2010-A unified model of short-range and long-range motion perception

13 0.89436603 128 nips-2010-Infinite Relational Modeling of Functional Connectivity in Resting State fMRI

14 0.89409178 44 nips-2010-Brain covariance selection: better individual functional connectivity models using population prior

15 0.89300013 268 nips-2010-The Neural Costs of Optimal Control

16 0.89231634 17 nips-2010-A biologically plausible network for the computation of orientation dominance

17 0.89026195 68 nips-2010-Effects of Synaptic Weight Diffusion on Learning in Decision Making Networks

18 0.8839975 127 nips-2010-Inferring Stimulus Selectivity from the Spatial Structure of Neural Network Dynamics

19 0.88097894 200 nips-2010-Over-complete representations on recurrent neural networks can support persistent percepts

20 0.87792802 19 nips-2010-A rational decision making framework for inhibitory control