nips nips2008 nips2008-231 nips2008-231-reference knowledge-graph by maker-knowledge-mining

231 nips-2008-Temporal Dynamics of Cognitive Control


Source: pdf

Author: Jeremy Reynolds, Michael C. Mozer

Abstract: Cognitive control refers to the flexible deployment of memory and attention in response to task demands and current goals. Control is often studied experimentally by presenting sequences of stimuli, some demanding a response, and others modulating the stimulus-response mapping. In these tasks, participants must maintain information about the current stimulus-response mapping in working memory. Prominent theories of cognitive control use recurrent neural nets to implement working memory, and optimize memory utilization via reinforcement learning. We present a novel perspective on cognitive control in which working memory representations are intrinsically probabilistic, and control operations that maintain and update working memory are dynamically determined via probabilistic inference. We show that our model provides a parsimonious account of behavioral and neuroimaging data, and suggest that it offers an elegant conceptualization of control in which behavior can be cast as optimal, subject to limitations on learning and the rate of information processing. Moreover, our model provides insight into how task instructions can be directly translated into appropriate behavior and then efficiently refined with subsequent task experience. 1


reference text

[1] E. K. Miller and J. D. Cohen. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24:167–202, 2001.

[2] A. Newell and H. A. Simon. Human Problem Solving. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, 1972.

[3] Robert D. Rogers and Stephen Monsell. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124:207– 231, 1995.

[4] Nick Yeung and Stephen Monsell. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 29(2):455–469, 2003.

[5] C. M. MacLeod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109:163–203, 1991.

[6] E. Koechlin, C. Ody, and F. Kouneiher. Neuroscience: The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science, 424:1181–1184, 2003.

[7] J. D. Cohen, K. Dunbar, and J. L. McClelland. On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97(3):332–361, 1990.

[8] S. J. Gilbert and T. Shallice. Task switching: A pdp model. Cognitive Psychology, 44:297–337, 2002.

[9] N. P. Rougier, D. Noelle, T. S. Braver, J. D. Cohen, and R. C. O’Reilly. Prefrontal cortex and the flexibility of cognitive control: Rules without symbols. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(20):7338–7343, 2005.

[10] M. J. Frank and R. C. O’Reilly. A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: Psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120:497–517, 2006.

[11] Stephen Monsell. Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce, editor, Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition, pages 93–148. Psychology press, Hove, UK, 1996.

[12] R S Zemel, P Dayan, and A Pouget. Probabilistic interpretation of population codes. Neural Comput, 10(2):403–430, 1998. 8