nips nips2007 nips2007-3 nips2007-3-reference knowledge-graph by maker-knowledge-mining

3 nips-2007-A Bayesian Model of Conditioned Perception


Source: pdf

Author: Alan Stocker, Eero P. Simoncelli

Abstract: unkown-abstract


reference text

[1] E.H. Adelson. Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. Science, 262:2042–2044, December 1993.

[2] S.P. Thompson. Optical illusions of motion. Brain, 3:289–298, 1880.

[3] S. Baldassi, N. Megna, and D.C. Burr. Visual clutter causes high-magnitude errors. PLoS Biology, 4(3):387ff, March 2006.

[4] M. Jazayeri and J.A. Movshon. A new perceptual illusion reveals mechanisms of sensory decoding. Nature, 446:912ff, April 2007.

[5] M.O. Ernst and M.S. Banks. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature, 415:429ff, January 2002.

[6] D. Brainard and W. Freeman. Bayesian color constancy. Journal of Optical Society of America A, 14(7):1393–1411, July 1997. 2 An example that is directly analogous to the perceptual experiment in [4] is documented in [18]: Subjects initially rated kitchen appliances for attractiveness, and then were allowed to select one as a gift from amongst two that they had rated equally. They were subsequently asked to rate the appliances again. The data show a repulsive bias of the post-decision ratings compared with the pre-decision ratings, such that the rating of the selected appliance increased, and the rating of the rejected appliance decreased. p(H) trial p(θ|H) mean 40 20 θ 0.2 A −β β −α −β β α 1/3 1/3 B 1/3 0 0 -20 -20 -40 -40 40 40 20 20 0 0 -20 α θ estimated direction [deg] 0.8 20 -20 −α 40 -40 -40 -20 -10 0 10 20 -20 -10 0 10 20 true direction [deg] Figure 6: Model predictions for two modifications of the original experiment. A: We change the prior probability p(H) to be asymmetric (0.8 vs. 0.2). However, we keep the prior distribution of motion directions given a particular side p(θ|H) constant within the range [0, ±α]. The model makes two predictions (trials shown for an intermediate coherence level): First, although tested with an equal number of trials for each motion direction, there is a strong bias induced by the asymmetric prior. And second, the direction estimates on the left are more veridical than on the right. B: We present two reference marks instead of one, asking the subjects to make a choice between three equally likely regions of motion direction. Again, we assume uniform prior distributions of motion directions within each area. The model predicts bilateral repulsion of the estimates in the central area, leading to a strong bias that is almost independent of coherence level.

[7] Y. Weiss, E. Simoncelli, and E. Adelson. Motion illusions as optimal percept. Nature Neuroscience, 5(6):598–604, June 2002.

[8] A.A. Stocker and E.P. Simoncelli. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception. Nature Neuroscience, pages 578–585, April 2006.

[9] D. Draper. Assessment and propagation of model uncertainty. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B, 57:45–97, 1995.

[10] J.A. Hoeting, D. Madigan, A.E. Raftery, and C.T. Volinsky. Bayesian model averaging: A tutorial. Statistical Science, 14(4):382–417, 1999.

[11] T.L. Griffiths, C. Kemp, and J. Tenenbaum. Handbook of Computational Cognitive Modeling, chapter Bayesian models of cognition. Cambridge University Press, to appear.

[12] J.A. Yu and P. Dayan. Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron, 46:681ff, May 2005.

[13] D. Knill. Robust cue integration: A Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant. Journal of Vision, 7(7):1–24, May 2007.

[14] K. K¨ rding and J. Tenenbaum. Causal inference in sensorimotor integration. In B. Sch¨ lkopf, J. Platt, o o and T. Hoffman, editors, Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 19. MIT Press, 2007.

[15] W.J. Ma, J.M. Beck, P.E. Latham, and A. Pouget. Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes. Nature Neuroscience, 9:1432ff, November 2006.

[16] Roitman J. D. Ditterich J. Mazurek, M. E. and M. N. Shadlen. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision-making. Cerebral Cortex, 13:1257–1269, 2003.

[17] L. Festinger. Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA, 1957.

[18] J.W. Brehm. Post-decision changes in the desirability of alternatives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52(3):384ff., 1956.